Computer-generated dot maps as an epidemiologic tool: investigating an outbreak of toxoplasmosis. (57/2878)

We used computer-generated dot maps to examine the spatial distribution of 94 Toxoplasma gondii infections associated with an outbreak in British Columbia, Canada. The incidence among patients served by one water distribution system was 3.52 times that of patients served by other sources. Acute T. gondii infection among 3, 812 pregnant women was associated with the incriminated distribution system.  (+info)

Optimal spatial frequencies for discrimination of motion direction in optic flow patterns. (58/2878)

Spatial frequency tuning functions were measured for direction discrimination of optic flow patterns. Three subjects discriminated the direction of a curved motion path using computer generated optic flow patterns composed of randomly positioned dots. Performance was measured with unfiltered patterns and with patterns that were spatially filtered across a range of spatial frequencies (center spatial frequencies of 0.4, 0.8, 1.6, 3.2, 6.4, and 9.6 c/deg). The same subjects discriminated the direction of uniform, translational motion on the fronto-parallel plane. The uniform motion patterns were also composed of randomly positioned dots, that were either unfiltered or filtered with the same spatial filters used for the optic flow patterns. The peak spatial frequency was the same for both the optic flow and uniform motion patterns. For both types of motion, a narrow band (1.5 octaves) of optimal spatial frequencies was sufficient to support the same level of performance as found with unfiltered, broadband patterns. Additional experiments demonstrated that the peak spatial frequency for the optic flow patterns varies with mean image speed in the same manner as has been reported for moving sinusoidal gratings. These findings confirm the hypothesis that the outputs of the local motion mechanisms thought to underlie the perception of uniform motion provide the inputs to, and constrain the operation of, the mechanism that processes self motion from optic flow patterns.  (+info)

Revisiting motion repulsion: evidence for a general phenomenon? (59/2878)

Previous studies have found large misperceptions when subjects are reporting the perceived angle between two directions of motion moving transparently at an acute angle, the so called motion repulsion. While these errors have been assumed to be caused by interactions between the two directions present, we reassessed these earlier measurements taking into account recent findings about directional misperceptions affecting the perception of single motion (reference repulsion). While our measurements confirm that errors in directional judgments of transparent motions can indeed be as big as 22 degrees we find that motion repulsion, i.e. the interaction between two directions, contributes at most about 7 degrees to these errors. This value is comparable to similar repulsion effects in orientation perception and stereoscopic depth perception, suggesting that they share a common neural basis. Our data further suggest that fast time scale adaptation and/or more general interactions between neurons contribute to motion repulsion while tracking eye movements play little or no role. These findings should serve as important constraints for models of motion perception.  (+info)

Flicker and the efficiency of cues for capturing attention. (60/2878)

In this paper, we present new experimental results which speak of the topic of temporal properties of processes underlying the selection of spatial location. We used the double motion induction paradigm to assess the strength of the selective effects. Prior exposure of an area to flicker, decreased the effectiveness of a cueing spot presented later at that location. This effect lasted for at least 1.5 s. In further experiments, it was found that both static and flickering cues, with time, lose their effectiveness to facilitate processing. While the static cueing decays quickly to very low effectiveness, flicker cueing decays to a level of effectiveness which can be maintained for a long time. Thus with time two flickering cues presented with a temporal offset become equivalent to each other, but remain more effective than a static cue. We conclude that mechanisms coding temporal change determine cue effectiveness for capturing attention. Simple exponential decay functions with different temporal constants and different lower asymptotes can describe these effects.  (+info)

Use of an early nonlinearity to measure optical and receptor resolution in the human infant. (61/2878)

We measured the resolution of the optics and receptoral processes in human infants. To do so, we recorded visual-evoked potentials (VEPs) to sampled sinewave gratings, stimuli that generate highly visible distortion products at a nonlinearity early in the retina. We varied the spatial frequency content of the stimulus to determine the frequencies that can be transmitted through the optics and receptors and thereby generate distortion products. Data were collected from adults and 2- to 7-month-old infants. The results indicated that the resolution of the infants' optical/receptoral processes was within a factor of two of adults' even at the earliest ages tested. These first stages of processing, therefore, do not explain infants' poor performance in many visual tasks, or restrict the types of visual stimuli affecting more central mechanisms that undergo experience-dependent development.  (+info)

Spatial-frequency bandwidth of perceived contrast. (62/2878)

The aim of this study was to investigate the spatial-frequency bandwidth of perceived suprathreshold contrast. It has been shown that for grating stimuli contrast detection thresholds depend on spatial frequency, grating area and the number of orientation components. However, suprathreshold contrast perception exhibits contrast constancy, i.e. suprathreshold contrast matches are independent of these stimulus parameters. To study whether contrast constancy applies to spatial-frequency bandwidth, contrast matching was performed and detection thresholds were measured for spatial noise stimuli at various bandwidths centred at 2 c/deg. At high contrast levels, contrast matches were nearly independent of stimulus spatial-frequency bandwidth up to about 6 octaves, even though detection thresholds increased with bandwidth. Thus, a broad band of spatial frequencies contributed to perceived suprathreshold contrast. The requisites for this are contrast constancy with respect to spatial frequency, and integration of contrast information across different spatial frequencies so that the effective bandwidth of the system is broad.  (+info)

Infants' sensitivity to statistical distributions of motion direction and speed. (63/2878)

Adults combine different local motions to form a global percept of motion. This study explores the origins of this process by testing how perturbations of local motion influence infants' sensitivity to global motion. Infants at 6-, 12-, and 18-weeks of age viewed random dots moving with a gaussian distribution of dot directions defined by a mean of 0 degree (rightward) or 180 degrees (leftward) and a standard deviation (SD) of 0, 34, or 68 degrees. A well-practiced observer used infants' optokinetic responses to judge the direction of stimulus motion. Infants were studied both cross-sectionally and longitudinally. Direction discrimination was relatively high at all ages when the SD was 0 degree. When the SD was 34 or 68 degrees, performance declined with age. Adult performance was nearly perfect at these SDs. A similar developmental pattern was found with distributions of dot speed. The decline in infant performance is consistent with the development of both neural tuning and receptive field size. The subsequent improvement by adulthood suggests the development of additional processes such as long-range interactions.  (+info)

The crystal structure of calcium-free human m-calpain suggests an electrostatic switch mechanism for activation by calcium. (64/2878)

Calpains (calcium-dependent cytoplasmic cysteine proteinases) are implicated in processes such as cytoskeleton remodeling and signal transduction. The 2.3-A crystal structure of full-length heterodimeric [80-kDa (dI-dIV) + 30-kDa (dV+dVI)] human m-calpain crystallized in the absence of calcium reveals an oval disc-like shape, with the papain-like catalytic domain dII and the two calmodulin-like domains dIV+dVI occupying opposite poles, and the tumor necrosis factor alpha-like beta-sandwich domain dIII and the N-terminal segments dI+dV located between. Compared with papain, the two subdomains dIIa+dIIb of the catalytic unit are rotated against one another by 50 degrees, disrupting the active site and the substrate binding site, explaining the inactivity of calpains in the absence of calcium. Calcium binding to an extremely negatively charged loop of domain dIII (an electrostatic switch) could release the adjacent barrel-like subdomain dIIb to move toward the helical subdomain dIIa, allowing formation of a functional catalytic center. This switch loop could also mediate membrane binding, thereby explaining calpains' strongly reduced calcium requirements in vivo. The activity status at the catalytic center might be further modulated by calcium binding to the calmodulin domains via the N-terminal linkers.  (+info)