Determination of the rate of the glutamate/glutamine cycle in the human brain by in vivo 13C NMR. (49/3458)

Recent 13C NMR studies in rat models have shown that the glutamate/glutamine cycle is highly active in the cerebral cortex and is coupled to incremental glucose oxidation in an approximately 1:1 stoichiometry. To determine whether a high level of glutamatergic activity is present in human cortex, the rates of the tricarboxylic acid cycle, glutamine synthesis, and the glutamate/glutamine cycle were determined in the human occipital/parietal lobe at rest. During an infusion of [1-13C]-glucose, in vivo 13C NMR spectra were obtained of the time courses of label incorporation into [4-13C]-glutamate and [4-13C]-glutamine. Using a metabolic model we have validated in the rat, we calculated a total tricarboxylic acid cycle rate of 0.77 +/- 0.07 micromol/min/g (mean +/- SD, n = 6), a glucose oxidation rate of 0.39 +/- 0.04 micromol/min/g, and a glutamate/glutamine cycle rate of 0.32 +/- 0.05 micromol/min/g (mean +/- SD, n = 6). In agreement with studies in rat cerebral cortex, the glutamate/glutamine cycle is a major metabolic flux in the resting human brain with a rate approximately 80% of glucose oxidation.  (+info)

Shape interactions in macaque inferior temporal neurons. (50/3458)

Missal et al. observed that the responses of inferior temporal (IT) neurons to a shape were reduced markedly when this shape partially overlapped a larger second shape, suggesting that shape interactions determine IT responses. In the present study, we compared the responses of IT neurons with combinations of two shapes which did or did not overlap and studied the effect of the relative and absolute positions of the two shapes. In a first test, a preferred shape (figure) was presented at the fixation point while a second, nonpreferred, shape was displayed either in the background of the figure (overlap) or at one of four peripheral positions (nonoverlap). Controls consisted of presentations of either shape in isolation at each of the five positions. The stimuli were presented during a fixation task. The responses to these combinations of two shapes were, on average, reduced compared with those elicited by the preferred shape presented in isolation. This suppression occurred whether or not the two shapes overlapped, but the degree of suppression in the overlap and nonoverlap conditions did not correlate. These interactions were stronger when the interacting stimulus was located in the contralateral compared with the ipsilateral hemifield. The position of the interacting stimulus within a hemifield significantly affected the suppression associated with combined shapes in some neurons. The strength of the interactions of the two nonoverlapping shapes depended on the shape of the interacting stimulus in half of the neurons. In a second test, the preferred shape and interacting stimulus could appear either at the fixation point or at one eccentric position. Here we found that the suppression was, on average, strongest when the interacting stimulus, rather than the preferred shape, was presented at the fixation position. Also, in 40% of the neurons, the response reduction was similar in overlap and nonoverlap conditions if effects of stimulus position were taken into account. In both tests, we also measured the responses to combinations of a nonpreferred shape and the interacting stimulus and showed that the response to a combination of two nonpreferred shapes was, in general, smaller than the response to a combination of the preferred and nonpreferred shape. Overall the results indicate that stimulus interactions in the receptive fields of IT neurons can be position and shape selective; this can contribute to the coding for the relationships between object parts.  (+info)

Functional anatomy of pursuit eye movements in humans as revealed by fMRI. (51/3458)

We have investigated the functional anatomy of pursuit eye movements in humans with functional magnetic imaging. The performance of pursuit eye movements induced activations in the cortical eye fields also activated during the execution of visually guided saccadic eye movements, namely in the precentral cortex [frontal eye field (FEF)], the medial superior frontal cortex (supplementary eye field), the intraparietal cortex (parietal eye field), and the precuneus, and at the junction of occipital and temporal cortex (MT/MST) cortex. Pursuit-related areas could be distinguished from saccade-related areas both in terms of spatial extent and location. Pursuit-related areas were smaller than their saccade-related counterparts, three of eight significantly so. The pursuit-related FEF was usually inferior to saccade-related FEF. Other pursuit-related areas were consistently posterior to their saccade-related counterparts. The current findings provide the first functional imaging evidence for a distinction between two parallel cortical systems that subserve pursuit and saccadic eye movements in humans.  (+info)

Alterations of muscarinic acetylcholine receptor subtypes in diffuse lewy body disease: relation to Alzheimer's disease. (52/3458)

OBJECTIVES: Dementia associated with Lewy bodies in cortical and subcortical areas is classified as dementia of the non-Alzheimer type and termed diffuse Lewy body disease (DLBD). The generic term "dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB)" was proposed in the international workshop on Lewy body dementia to include the similar disorders presenting Lewy bodies. In DLB, a lower level of choline acetyltransferase (ChAT) activity in the neocortex was found compared with that in Alzheimer's disease. The purpose of the present study was to determine the total amount of muscarinic acetylcholine receptors (mAChRs) and relative proportion of each subtype (m1-m4) of mAChRs in the frontal and temporal cortex of seven DLBD and 11 Alzheimer's disease necropsied brains. METHODS: A [(3)H]quinuclidinyl benzilate (QNB) binding assay and an immunoprecipitation assay using subtype-specific antibodies were performed. Each antibody was raised against fusion proteins containing peptides corresponding to the third intracellular (i3) loops of the respective mAChR subtype. RESULTS: The total amounts of mAChRs were significantly lower in the preparations of temporal cortices from DLBD and Alzheimer's disease than in those from dead controls (seven cases). In both diseases, the proportion of the m3 receptor in the frontal cortex was significantly increased and that of the m4 receptor in the temporal cortex was significantly decreased compared with the control specimens. The proportions of the m1 and m2 subtypes were significantly different in the temporal cortex. The proportion of the m1 receptor was significantly greater in the DLBD brains, whereas that of the m2 receptor was significantly greater in the Alzheimer's disease brains than in the controls. CONCLUSIONS: The m1 receptor is the major subtype in the cerebral cortex, and m2 is known to be present at presynaptic terminals. The higher proportions of m1 in DLBD and m2 in Alzheimer's disease suggest that the manner of degeneration in the cholinergic system is different between the diseases. It is hypothesised that a severe depletion of presynaptic cholinergic projective neurons causes the upregulation of m1 receptor in the temporal cortex in DLBD.  (+info)

The contribution of color to motion processing in Macaque middle temporal area. (53/3458)

The chromatic properties of an image yield strong cues for object boundaries and thus hold the potential to facilitate the detection of object motion. The extent to which cortical motion detectors exploit chromatic information, however, remains a matter of debate. To address this further, we quantified the strength of chromatic input to directionally selective neurons in the middle temporal area (MT) of macaque cerebral cortex using an equivalent luminance contrast (EqLC) paradigm. This paradigm, in which two sinusoidal gratings, one heterochromatic and the other achromatic, are superimposed and moved in opposite directions, allows the sensitivity of motion detectors to heterochromatic stimuli to be quantified and expressed relative to the benchmark of sensitivity for a luminance-defined stimulus. The results of these experiments demonstrate that the chromatic contrast in a moving red-green heterochromatic grating strongly influences directional responses in MT when the luminance contrast in that same grating is relatively low; for such stimuli, EqLC is at least 5%. When luminance contrast is added to the heterochromatic grating, however, EqLC wanes sharply and becomes negative (-4%) when luminance contrast is sufficiently high (>17-23%). Thus, the chromatic properties of an object appear to confer little or no benefit to motion processing by MT neurons when sufficient luminance contrast concurrently exists. These data support a simple model in which chromatic motion processing in MT is almost exclusively determined by magnocellular input. Additionally, a comparison of neuronal and psychophysical data suggests that MT may not be the sole contributor to the perceptual experience elicited by motion of heterochromatic patterns, or that only a subset of MT neurons serve this function.  (+info)

Cognitive asymmetry patterns in schizophrenia: active and withdrawn syndromes and sex differences as moderators. (54/3458)

Recognition memory for words and faces was examined in male and female schizophrenia patients for evidence of associations between putative left-right hemisphere asymmetry patterns and active (positive) versus withdrawn (negative) syndromes. Ninety-five normal controls and 104 schizophrenia patients with active, withdrawn, and mixed syndromes or in symptom remission were examined, including an unmedicated subgroup. Memory was poorer in patients than controls, while the remitted group had superior memory to psychotic patients. Active and withdrawn patients showed the hypothesized syndrome-dependent cognitive asymmetries: active (word > faces); withdrawn (faces > words), except active females who showed a word deficit. The results support selective lateralized temporoparietal impairment of either hemisphere in schizophrenia, with laterality related to active (face memory/right-sided impairment) and withdrawn (word memory/left-sided impairment) syndromes, except active syndrome females. These syndrome-related asymmetries moderated the sexually dimorphic asymmetries found in normal subjects. Consideration of individual differences both in sex and syndromes based on activity and withdrawal, and of left and right hemisphere memory modality, may assist in unraveling heterogeneity in schizophrenic cognition. The superior memory of recovered patients indicates that some memory impairment in schizophrenia is functional.  (+info)

Quantifying variability in the planum temporale: a probability map. (55/3458)

The acquisition of definitive evidence for systematic hemispheric asymmetries in the size of the planum temporale (PT) has been restricted by difficulties in identifying, standardizing and measuring the region of interest. In this paper an operational definition for identifying the problematic posterior border of the PT on magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans is proposed. An interactive voxel-painting program was used to identify and label the PT simultaneously in horizontal, sagittal and coronal planes in MRI scans, transformed into the standardized Talairach-Tournoux stereo-taxic space, from 50 normal right-handed volunteers. Both grey matter volume and cortical surface area of the PT were measured, while controlling for individual variation in overall brain shape and volume. The labeled tissue was averaged together to produce a probability map in standardized space of the region of interest. The PT region is highly variable, with no single voxel being labeled with a probability of >65%. In this study there were no significant hemispheric differences in volume or area of the PT. An asymmetry in area and volume was introduced by using an alternative method - the 'knife-cut' method - for identifying the posterior border. Implications for functional neuroimaging of the PT are discussed.  (+info)

Distributed representation of objects in the human ventral visual pathway. (56/3458)

Brain imaging and electrophysiological recording studies in humans have reported discrete cortical regions in posterior ventral temporal cortex that respond preferentially to faces, buildings, and letters. These findings suggest a category-specific anatomically segregated modular organization of the object vision pathway. Here we present data from a functional MRI study in which we found three distinct regions of ventral temporal cortex that responded preferentially to faces and two categories of other objects, namely houses and chairs, and had a highly consistent topological arrangement. Although the data could be interpreted as evidence for separate modules, we found that each category also evoked significant responses in the regions that responded maximally to other stimuli. Moreover, each category was associated with its own differential pattern of response across ventral temporal cortex. These results indicate that the representation of an object is not restricted to a region that responds maximally to that object, but rather is distributed across a broader expanse of cortex. We propose that the functional architecture of the ventral visual pathway is not a mosaic of category-specific modules but instead is a continuous representation of information about object form that has a highly consistent and orderly topological arrangement.  (+info)