Manual tracking in two dimensions. (17/829)

Manual tracking was studied by asking subjects to follow, with their finger, a target moving on a touch-sensitive video monitor. The target initially moved in a straight line at a constant speed and then, at a random point in time, made one abrupt change in direction. The results were approximated with a simple model according to which, after a reaction time, the hand moved in a straight line to intercept the target. Both the direction of hand motion and its peak speed could be predicted by assuming a constant time to intercept. This simple model was able to account for results obtained over a broad range of target speeds as well as the results of experiments in which both the speed and the direction of the target changed simultaneously. The results of an experiment in which the target acceleration was nonzero suggested that the error signals used during tracking are related to both speed and direction but poorly (if at all) to target acceleration. Finally, in an experiment in which target velocity remained constant along one axis but the perpendicular component underwent a step change, tracking along both axes was perturbed. This last finding demonstrates that tracking in two dimensions cannot be decomposed into its Cartesian components. However, an analytical model in a hand-centered frame of reference in which speed and direction are the controlled variables could account for much of the data.  (+info)

Neck muscle vibration makes walking humans accelerate in the direction of gaze. (18/829)

We studied the effect of the continuous vibration of symmetrical dorsal neck muscles in seven normal subjects during (a) quiet standing, (b) stepping in place movements and (c) walking on the treadmill. The experiments were performed in a darkened room and the subjects were given the instruction not to resist the applied perturbation. In one condition the velocity of the treadmill was controlled by feedback from the subject's current position. Head, trunk and leg motion were recorded at 100 Hz. In normal standing, neck vibration elicited a prominent forward body sway. During stepping in place, neck vibration produced an involuntary forward stepping at about 0.3 m s-1 without modifying the stepping frequency. If the head was turned horizontally 45 and 90 deg to the right or to the left, neck muscle vibration caused stepping approximately in the direction of the head naso-occipital axis. For lateral eye deviations, the direction of stepping was roughly aligned with gaze direction. In treadmill locomotion, neck vibration produced an involuntary step-like increase of walking speed (by 0.1-0.6 m s-1), independent of the initial walking speed. During backward locomotion, the walking speed tended to decrease during neck vibration. Thus, continuous neck vibration evokes changes in the postural reference during quiet standing and in the walking speed during locomotion. The results suggest that the proprioceptive input from the neck is integrated in the control of human posture and locomotion and is processed in the context of a viewer-centred reference frame.  (+info)

Apparent motion produces multiple deficits in visually guided smooth pursuit eye movements of monkeys. (19/829)

We used apparent motion targets to explore how degraded visual motion alters smooth pursuit eye movements. Apparent motion targets consisted of brief stationary flashes with a spatial separation (Deltax), temporal separation (Deltat), and apparent target velocity equal to Deltax/Deltat. Changes in pursuit initiation were readily observed when holding target velocity constant and increasing the flash separation. As flash separation increased, the first deficit observed was an increase in the latency to peak eye acceleration. Also seen was a paradoxical increase in initial eye acceleration. Further increases in the flash separation produced larger increases in latency and resulted in decreased eye acceleration. By varying target velocity, we were able to discern that the visual inputs driving pursuit initiation show both temporal and spatial limits. For target velocities above 4-8 degrees /s, deficits in the initiation of pursuit were seen when Deltax exceeded 0.2-0.5 degrees, even when Deltat was small. For target velocities below 4-8 degrees /s, deficits appeared when Deltat exceeded 32-64 ms, even when Deltax was small. Further experiments were designed to determine whether the spatial limit varied as retinal and extra-retinal factors changed. Varying the initial retinal position of the target for motion at 18 degrees /s revealed that the spatial limit increased as a function of retinal eccentricity. We then employed targets that increased velocity twice, once from fixation and again during pursuit. These experiments revealed that, as expected, the spatial limit is expressed in terms of the flash separation on the retina. The spatial limit is uninfluenced by either eye velocity or the absolute velocity of the target. These experiments also demonstrate that "initiation" deficits can be observed during ongoing pursuit, and are thus not deficits in initiation per se. We conclude that such deficits result from degradation of the retino-centric motion signals that drive pursuit eye acceleration. For large flash separations, we also observed deficits in the maintenance of pursuit: sustained eye velocity failed to match the constant apparent target velocity. Deficits in the maintenance of pursuit depended on both target velocity and Deltat and did not result simply from a failure of degraded image motion signals to drive eye acceleration. We argue that such deficits result from a low gain in the eye velocity memory that normally supports the maintenance of pursuit. This low gain may appear because visual inputs are so degraded that the transition from fixation to tracking is incomplete.  (+info)

Early components of the human vestibulo-ocular response to head rotation: latency and gain. (20/829)

To characterize vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) properties in the time window in which contributions by other systems are minimal, eye movements during the first 50-100 ms after the start of transient angular head accelerations ( approximately 1000 degrees /s(2)) imposed by a torque helmet were analyzed in normal human subjects. Orientations of the head and both eyes were recorded with magnetic search coils (resolution, approximately 1 min arc; 1000 samples/s). Typically, the first response to a head perturbation was an anti-compensatory eye movement with zero latency, peak-velocity of several degrees per second, and peak excursion of several tenths of a degree. This was interpreted as a passive mechanical response to linear acceleration of the orbital tissues caused by eccentric rotation of the eye. The response was modeled as a damped oscillation (approximately 13 Hz) of the orbital contents, approaching a constant eye deviation for a sustained linear acceleration. The subsequent compensatory eye movements showed (like the head movements) a linear increase in velocity, which allowed estimates of latency and gain with linear regressions. After appropriate accounting for the preceding passive eye movements, average VOR latency (for pooled eyes, directions, and subjects) was calculated as 8.6 ms. Paired comparisons between the two eyes revealed that the latency for the eye contralateral to the direction of head rotation was, on average, 1.3 ms shorter than for the ipsilateral eye. This highly significant average inter-ocular difference was attributed to the additional internuclear abducens neuron in the pathway to the ipsilateral eye. Average acceleration gain (ratio between slopes of eye and head velocities) over the first 40-50 ms was approximately 1.1. Instantaneous velocity gain, calculated as Veye(t)/Vhead(t-latency), showed a gradual build-up converging toward unity (often after a slight overshoot). Instantaneous acceleration gain also converged toward unity but showed a much steeper build-up and larger oscillations. This behavior of acceleration and velocity gain could be accounted for by modeling the eye movements as the sum of the passive response to the linear acceleration and the active rotational VOR. Due to the latency and the anticompensatory component, gaze stabilization was never complete. The influence of visual targets was limited. The initial VOR was identical with a distant target (continuously visible or interrupted) and in complete darkness. A near visual target caused VOR gain to rise to a higher level, but the time after which the difference between far and near targets emerged varied between individuals.  (+info)

Coactivation of the antagonist muscle does not covary with steadiness in old adults. (21/829)

The purpose of the study was to determine the association between steadiness and activation of the agonist and antagonist muscles during isometric and anisometric contractions. Young (n = 14) and old (n = 15) adults used the first dorsal interosseus muscle to perform constant-force and constant-load tasks (2.5, 5, 20, 50, and 75% maximum) with the left index finger. Steadiness was quantified as the coefficient of variation of force and the SD of acceleration normalized to the load lifted. The old adults were less steady at most target forces with isometric contractions (2.5, 5, and 50%) and with most loads during the anisometric contractions (2.5, 5, and 20%). Furthermore, the old adults were less steady when performing lengthening contractions (up to 50%) compared with shortening contractions, whereas there was no difference for young adults. The reduced steadiness exhibited by the old adults during these tasks was not associated with differences in the average level of agonist muscle electromyogram or with coactivation of the antagonist muscle.  (+info)

Spatiotemporal processing of linear acceleration: primary afferent and central vestibular neuron responses. (22/829)

Spatiotemporal convergence and two-dimensional (2-D) neural tuning have been proposed as a major neural mechanism in the signal processing of linear acceleration. To examine this hypothesis, we studied the firing properties of primary otolith afferents and central otolith neurons that respond exclusively to horizontal linear accelerations of the head (0.16-10 Hz) in alert rhesus monkeys. Unlike primary afferents, the majority of central otolith neurons exhibited 2-D spatial tuning to linear acceleration. As a result, central otolith dynamics vary as a function of movement direction. During movement along the maximum sensitivity direction, the dynamics of all central otolith neurons differed significantly from those observed for the primary afferent population. Specifically at low frequencies (+info)

Parallels between timing of onset responses of single neurons in cat and of evoked magnetic fields in human auditory cortex. (23/829)

Sound onsets constitute particularly salient transients and evoke strong responses from neurons of the auditory system, but in the past, such onset responses have often been analyzed with respect to steady-state features of sounds, like the sound pressure level. Recent electrophysiological studies of single neurons from the auditory cortex of anesthetized cats have revealed that the timing and strength of onset responses are shaped by dynamic stimulus properties at their very onsets. Here we demonstrate with magnetoencephalography that stimulus-response relationships very similar to those of the single neurons are observed in two onset components, N100m and P50m, of auditory evoked magnetic fields (AEFs) from the auditory cortex of awake humans. In response to tones shaped with cosine-squared rise functions, N100m and P50m peak latencies vary systematically with tone level and rise time but form a rather invariant function of the acceleration of the envelope at tone onset. Hence N100m and P50m peak latencies, as well as peak amplitudes, are determined by dynamic properties of the stimuli within the first few milliseconds, though not necessarily by acceleration. The changes of N100m and P50m peak latencies with rise time and level are incompatible with a fixed-amplitude threshold model. The direct comparison of the neuromagnetic and single-neuron data shows that, on average, the variance of the neuromagnetic data is larger by one to two orders of magnitude but that favorable measurements can yield variances as low as those derived from neurons with mediocre precision of response timing. The striking parallels between the response timing of single cortical neurons and of AEFs provides a stronger link between single neuron and population activity.  (+info)

Noninvasive motion ventilation (NIMV): a novel approach to ventilatory support. (24/829)

A motion platform was developed that oscillates an animal in a foot-to-head direction (z-plane). The platform varies the frequency and intensity of acceleration, imparting periodic sinusoidal inertial forces (pG(z)) to the body. The aim of the study was to characterize ventilation produced by the noninvasive motion ventilator (NIMV) in animals with healthy and diseased lungs. Incremental increases in pG(z) (acceleration) with the frequency held constant (f = 4 Hz) produced almost linear increases in minute ventilation (VE). Frequencies of 2-4 Hz produced the greatest VE and tidal volume (VT) for any given acceleration between +/-0.2 and +/-0.8 G. Increasing the force due to acceleration produced proportional increases in both transpulmonary and transdiaphragmatic pressures. Increasing transpulmonary pressure by increasing pG(z) produced linear increases in VT, similar to spontaneous breathing. NIMV reversed deliberately induced hypoventilation and normalized the changes in arterial blood gases induced by meconium aspiration. In conclusion, a novel motion platform is described that imparts periodic sinusoidal acceleration forces at moderate frequencies (4 Hz) to the whole body in the z-plane. These forces, when properly adjusted, are capable of highly effective ventilation of normal and diseased lungs. Such noninvasive ventilation is accomplished at airway pressures equivalent to atmospheric or continuous positive airway pressure, with acceleration forces less than +/-1 G(z).  (+info)