Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. (57/3615)

Structural MRIs of the brains of humans with extensive navigation experience, licensed London taxi drivers, were analyzed and compared with those of control subjects who did not drive taxis. The posterior hippocampi of taxi drivers were significantly larger relative to those of control subjects. A more anterior hippocampal region was larger in control subjects than in taxi drivers. Hippocampal volume correlated with the amount of time spent as a taxi driver (positively in the posterior and negatively in the anterior hippocampus). These data are in accordance with the idea that the posterior hippocampus stores a spatial representation of the environment and can expand regionally to accommodate elaboration of this representation in people with a high dependence on navigational skills. It seems that there is a capacity for local plastic change in the structure of the healthy adult human brain in response to environmental demands.  (+info)

Sparse-sampling of gratings in the visual cortex of strabismic amblyopes. (58/3615)

Strabismic amblyopes show losses in positional acuity that cannot be explained by their resolution or contrast sensitivities. One hypothesis for these losses is a reduction in the density of cortical neurons that are driven by the amblyopic eye (cortical undersampling). The question this study addressed was whether the foveal representation of the amblyopic eye is undersampled in the cortex of strabismic amblyopes. In order to assess spatial sampling psychophysically, we recorded the perceived orientation of a stationary grating as a function of grating orientation and frequency in three strabismic amblyopes. To ensure high retinal contrast, the grating was imaged on the fovea of each observer using a laser interferometer. We found that the strabismic amblyopes misperceived the orientation of the grating at spatial frequencies that are a factor of two to six lower than the sampling frequency of the foveal cones. Since the retina and LGN in strabismic amblyopes are presumably normal, this result suggests sparse cortical sampling in the foveal representation of the amblyopic eye. Undersampling by cortical neurons may contribute to the spatial distortions present in strabismic amblyopic eyes.  (+info)

The neural deficit in strabismic amblyopia: sampling considerations. (59/3615)

In order to understand the nature of the neural loss in strabismic amblyopia, we have applied a technique which has been used in the normal periphery to psychophysically probe the sampling properties of the neuronal population. We ask whether there is a 'sampling' deficit and if so whether it is based on either an absolute loss of neurons (i.e. spatial undersampling) or an irregular arrangement of a normal number of neurons (i.e. irregular sampling). Our results suggest that neural pooling restricts the spatial frequency region where sampling considerations are important to a very small part of the visible high spatial frequency range. Within this limited region, irregular sampling rather than spatial undersampling is the greater contributor to the strabismic amblyope deficit.  (+info)

Predicting the present direction of heading. (60/3615)

Humans perceive heading accurately when they rotate their eyes. This is remarkable, because (1) the pursuit eye movement makes the retinal flow more complicated; and (2) the eye rotation causes a continuous change of the heading direction on the retina. The first problem prevents a simple association of the centre of flow on the retina with the heading direction. To solve it, the brain needs to take into account the flow associated with the eye's rotation. But even if this is done correctly, the resulting estimate of the heading is retino-centric and changing over time. Thus, the processing time to retrieve the heading from the flow field will cause a lag with respect to the actual heading direction. We investigated the latency for heading perception. We presented step wise changes of the centre of expanding flow to stationary and moving eyes. This mimics the movement of the heading direction across the retina, but avoids the complicating effects of rotational flow. For a stationary eye, we found a bias in perceived heading that corresponds to a latency of 300 ms or more. Yet, errors in heading perception are marginal normally, because we found an opposite bias for the moving eye, which counters the errors due to latency and a changing retino-centric heading direction. This suggests that the current heading direction is predicted from the extra-retinal signal and the delayed visual signals.  (+info)

Visual adaptation as optimal information transmission. (61/3615)

We propose that visual adaptation in orientation, spatial frequency, and motion can be understood from the perspective of optimal information transmission. The essence of the proposal is that neural response properties at the system level should be adjusted to the changing statistics of the input so as to maximize information transmission. We show that this principle accounts for several well-documented psychophysical phenomena, including the tilt aftereffect, change in contrast sensitivity and post-adaptation changes in orientation discrimination. Adaptation can also be considered on a longer time scale, in the context of tailoring response properties to natural scene statistics. From the anisotropic distribution of power in natural scenes, the proposal also predicts differences in the contrast sensitivity function across spatial frequency and orientation, including the oblique effect.  (+info)

Integration of foveal orientation signals: distinct local and long-range spatial domains. (62/3615)

Human observers can discriminate the orientation of a stimulus configuration composed of a pair of collinear visual patterns much better than that of a single component pattern alone. Previous investigations of this type of orientation signal integration and of other similar visual integrative functions have shown that, for closely spaced elements, there is integration only for stimuli with the same contrast polarity (i.e., both lighter or both darker than the background) but, at greater separations, integration is independent of contrast polarity. Is this effect specific to differences in contrast polarity, which is known to be an important parameter in the organization of the visual system, or might there be a cluster of other stimulus dimensions that show similar effects, indicating a more widespread distinction between the processes limiting integration at local and long-range spatial scales? Here, we report a similar distance dependence for orientation signal integration across stimulus differences in binocular disparity, direction of motion, and direction of figure-ground assignment. We also demonstrate that the selectivity found at short separations cannot be explained only by "end-cuts," the small borders created at the junction of abutting contrasting patterns. These findings imply the existence of two distinct spatial domains for the integration of foveal orientation information: a local zone in which integration is highly selective for a number of salient stimulus parameters and a long-range domain in which integration is relatively unselective and only requires that patterns be roughly collinear.  (+info)

Plasticity in adult cat visual cortex (area 17) following circumscribed monocular lesions of all retinal layers. (63/3615)

1. In eight adult cats intense, sharply circumscribed, monocular laser lesions were used to remove all cellular layers of the retina. The extents of the retinal lesions were subsequently confirmed with counts of alpha-ganglion cells in retinal whole mounts; in some cases these revealed radial segmental degeneration of ganglion cells distal to the lesion. 2. Two to 24 weeks later, area 17 (striate cortex; V1) was studied electrophysiologically in a standard anaesthetized, paralysed (artificially respired) preparation. Recording single- or multineurone activity revealed extensive topographical reorganization within the lesion projection zone (LPZ). 3. Thus, with stimulation of the lesioned eye, about 75 % of single neurones in the LPZ had 'ectopic' visual discharge fields which were displaced to normal retina in the immediate vicinity of the lesion. 4. The sizes of the ectopic discharge fields were not significantly different from the sizes of the normal discharge fields. Furthermore, binocular cells recorded from the LPZ, when stimulated via their ectopic receptive fields, exhibited orientation tuning and preferred stimulus velocities which were indistinguishable from those found when the cells were stimulated via the normal eye. 5. However, the responses to stimuli presented via ectopic discharge fields were generally weaker (lower peak discharge rates) than those to presentations via normal discharge fields, and were characterized by a lower-than-normal upper velocity limit. 6. Overall, the properties of the ectopic receptive fields indicate that cortical mechanisms rather than a retinal 'periphery' effect underlie the topographic reorganization of area 17 following monocular retinal lesions.  (+info)

Speed selectivity for optic flow in area 7a of the behaving macaque. (64/3615)

Area 7a, in the inferior parietal lobe, has been implicated in optic flow processing to obtain spatial information about the environment. Optic flow, angle-of-gaze and center-of-motion dependencies are already documented, but the selectivity of area 7a to speed is unknown. Such information is crucial as area 7a provides the final step in visual motion analysis that begins at the lateral geniculate nucleus and passes through MT, MST and LIP/VIP. Macaque area 7a neurons were tested with optic flows with speeds of 0.5-128 degrees /s. Of 161 neurons tested in four hemispheres of two adult male macaques, 53% (86/161) were speed selective at either the time of stimulus onset, at the end of the trial, or at both times. Speed selec- tivities resembling the basic filter types (band-pass, band-reject, high-pass, low-pass, broadband) were found. Area 7a neurons exhibited two novel properties not previously reported elsewhere. Speed selectivity was found to be dynamic in that many cells gained, lost or changed speed tuning over the course of a trial. In addition, speed dependence and optic flow selectivity interacted. For example, a cell could preferentially respond to one type of naviga- tional optic flow at a slow speed and a different navigational optic flow at a fast speed. The presence of speed selectivity combined with other properties of area 7a neurons indicates that these neurons may have a role in the concurrent representation of heading as well as multiple object speeds and directions.  (+info)